Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 169
Filtrar
1.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 11628, 2023 07 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37468709

RESUMO

Saccades change eye position and interrupt vision several times per second, necessitating neural mechanisms for continuous perception of object identity, orientation, and location. Neuroimaging studies suggest that occipital and parietal cortex play complementary roles for transsaccadic perception of intrinsic versus extrinsic spatial properties, e.g., dorsomedial occipital cortex (cuneus) is sensitive to changes in spatial frequency, whereas the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) is modulated by changes in object orientation. Based on this, we hypothesized that both structures would be recruited to simultaneously monitor object identity and orientation across saccades. To test this, we merged two previous neuroimaging protocols: 21 participants viewed a 2D object and then, after sustained fixation or a saccade, judged whether the shape or orientation of the re-presented object changed. We, then, performed a bilateral region-of-interest analysis on identified cuneus and SMG sites. As hypothesized, cuneus showed both saccade and feature (i.e., object orientation vs. shape change) modulations, and right SMG showed saccade-feature interactions. Further, the cuneus activity time course correlated with several other cortical saccade/visual areas, suggesting a 'functional network' for feature discrimination. These results confirm the involvement of occipital/parietal cortex in transsaccadic vision and support complementary roles in spatial versus identity updating.


Assuntos
Lobo Parietal , Movimentos Sacádicos , Humanos , Percepção , Lobo Occipital , Neuroimagem
2.
Neuroimage ; 197: 306-319, 2019 08 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31051295

RESUMO

Movement planning involves transforming the sensory signals into a command in motor coordinates. Surprisingly, the real-time dynamics of sensorimotor transformations at the whole brain level remain unknown, in part due to the spatiotemporal limitations of fMRI and neurophysiological recordings. Here, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) during pro-/anti-wrist pointing to determine (1) the cortical areas involved in transforming visual signals into appropriate hand motor commands, and (2) how this transformation occurs in real time, both within and across the regions involved. We computed sensory, motor, and sensorimotor indices in 16 bilateral brain regions for direction coding based on hemispherically lateralized de/synchronization in the α (7-15 Hz) and ß (15-35 Hz) bands. We found a visuomotor progression, from pure sensory codes in 'early' occipital-parietal areas, to a temporal transition from sensory to motor coding in the majority of parietal-frontal sensorimotor areas, to a pure motor code, in both the α and ß bands. Further, the timing of these transformations revealed a top-down pro/anti cue influence that propagated 'backwards' from frontal through posterior cortical areas. These data directly demonstrate a progressive, real-time transformation both within and across the entire occipital-parietal-frontal network that follows specific rules of spatial distribution and temporal order.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Sincronização Cortical , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Magnetoencefalografia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Punho , Adulto Jovem
3.
Eur J Neurol ; 26(9): 1161-1167, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30927497

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: The Vascular Behavioral and Cognitive Disorders (VASCOG) criteria for vascular cognitive disorders were published in 2014, but their concurrent and predictive validity have not been examined. METHODS: Participants (N = 165, aged 49-86 years) were from Sydney Stroke Study, a longitudinal study of post-stroke cognitive impairment and dementia. Diagnoses using the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke - Association Internationale pour la Recherché et l'Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINDS-AIREN), the Alzheimer's Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers (ADDTC) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV), criteria for vascular dementia (VaD) were made by consensus at multidisciplinary case conferences. Diagnoses for mild vascular cognitive disorder (mVCD) and VaD using VASCOG, DSM-5 and the Vascular Impairment of Cognition Classification Consensus Study (VICCCS) criteria were made by two study authors. Agreement levels between criteria sets were examined using Cohen's kappa (κ). The ability of VaD diagnoses to predict mortality over 10 years and of mVCD to predict dementia over 5 years was investigated. RESULTS: The VASCOG criteria yielded rates of mVCD slightly lower than for DSM-5 and VICCCS. VaD rates were similar for all criteria, although slightly lower for DSM-IV. Agreement between the VASCOG, VICCCS and DSM-5 criteria was excellent for VaD and mVCD (κ = 0.83-1.0), but lower for VaD between VASCOG and the other criteria (κ = 0.47-0.63). VaD-based mortality predictions were similar for the VASCOG, VICCCS and DSM-5 criteria, and higher than those for other criteria. The prediction of incident dementia within 5 years from mVCD was slightly lower with VASCOG criteria than with DSM-5 and VICCCS criteria. CONCLUSIONS: The VASCOG criteria have greater sensitivity, modest concurrent validity and better predictive validity than older criteria for VaD, but are comparable to DSM-5 and VICCCS criteria. Their operationalization and inclusion of a mild VCD category make them useful for clinical and research applications.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Demência Vascular/diagnóstico , Guias de Prática Clínica como Assunto/normas , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/complicações , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Disfunção Cognitiva/etiologia , Demência Vascular/etiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
4.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 11: 44, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28690501

RESUMO

A remembered saccade target could be encoded in egocentric coordinates such as gaze-centered, or relative to some external allocentric landmark that is independent of the target or gaze (landmark-centered). In comparison to egocentric mechanisms, very little is known about such a landmark-centered representation. Here, we used an event-related fMRI design to identify brain areas supporting these two types of spatial coding (i.e., landmark-centered vs. gaze-centered) for target memory during the Delay phase where only target location, not saccade direction, was specified. The paradigm included three tasks with identical display of visual stimuli but different auditory instructions: Landmark Saccade (remember target location relative to a visual landmark, independent of gaze), Control Saccade (remember original target location relative to gaze fixation, independent of the landmark), and a non-spatial control, Color Report (report target color). During the Delay phase, the Control and Landmark Saccade tasks activated overlapping areas in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and frontal cortex as compared to the color control, but with higher activation in PPC for target coding in the Control Saccade task and higher activation in temporal and occipital cortex for target coding in Landmark Saccade task. Gaze-centered directional selectivity was observed in superior occipital gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, whereas landmark-centered directional selectivity was observed in precuneus and midposterior intraparietal sulcus. During the Response phase after saccade direction was specified, the parietofrontal network in the left hemisphere showed higher activation for rightward than leftward saccades. Our results suggest that cortical activation for coding saccade target direction relative to a visual landmark differs from gaze-centered directional selectivity for target memory, from the mechanisms for other types of allocentric tasks, and from the directionally selective mechanisms for saccade planning and execution.

5.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 10: 415, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27582701

RESUMO

The limited capacity of visual working memory (VWM) necessitates attentional mechanisms that selectively update and maintain only the most task-relevant content. Psychophysical experiments have shown that the retroactive selection of memory content can be based on visual properties such as location or shape, but the neural basis for such differential selection is unknown. For example, it is not known if there are different cortical modules specialized for spatial vs. feature-based mnemonic attention, in the same way that has been demonstrated for attention to perceptual input. Here, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to identify areas in human parietal and occipital cortex involved in the selection of objects from memory based on cues to their location (spatial information) or their shape (featural information). We found that TMS over the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) selectively facilitated spatial selection, whereas TMS over the lateral occipital cortex (LO) selectively enhanced feature-based selection for remembered objects in the contralateral visual field. Thus, different cortical regions are responsible for spatial vs. feature-based selection of working memory representations. Since the same regions are involved in terms of attention to external events, these new findings indicate overlapping mechanisms for attentional control over perceptual input and mnemonic representations.

6.
Neuropsychologia ; 63: 185-93, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25192630

RESUMO

Humans typically make several rapid eye movements (saccades) per second. It is thought that visual working memory can retain and spatially integrate three to four objects or features across each saccade but little is known about this neural mechanism. Previously we showed that transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to the posterior parietal cortex and frontal eye fields degrade trans-saccadic memory of multiple object features (Prime, Vesia, & Crawford, 2008, Journal of Neuroscience, 28(27), 6938-6949; Prime, Vesia, & Crawford, 2010, Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 759-772.). Here, we used a similar protocol to investigate whether dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), an area involved in spatial working memory, is also involved in trans-saccadic memory. Subjects were required to report changes in stimulus orientation with (saccade task) or without (fixation task) an eye movement in the intervening memory interval. We applied single-pulse TMS to left and right DLPFC during the memory delay, timed at three intervals to arrive approximately 100 ms before, 100 ms after, or at saccade onset. In the fixation task, left DLPFC TMS produced inconsistent results, whereas right DLPFC TMS disrupted performance at all three intervals (significantly for presaccadic TMS). In contrast, in the saccade task, TMS consistently facilitated performance (significantly for left DLPFC/perisaccadic TMS and right DLPFC/postsaccadic TMS) suggesting a dis-inhibition of trans-saccadic processing. These results are consistent with a neural circuit of trans-saccadic memory that overlaps and interacts with, but is partially separate from the circuit for visual working memory during sustained fixation.


Assuntos
Memória/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto Jovem
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 24(6): 1540-54, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23362111

RESUMO

Grasping behaviors require the selection of grasp-relevant object dimensions, independent of overall object size. Previous neuroimaging studies found that the intraparietal cortex processes object size, but it is unknown whether the graspable dimension (i.e., grasp axis between selected points on the object) or the overall size of objects triggers activation in that region. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation to investigate human brain areas involved in processing the grasp-relevant dimension of real 3-dimensional objects in grasping and viewing tasks. Trials consisted of 2 sequential stimuli in which the object's grasp-relevant dimension, its global size, or both were novel or repeated. We found that calcarine and extrastriate visual areas adapted to object size regardless of the grasp-relevant dimension during viewing tasks. In contrast, the superior parietal occipital cortex (SPOC) and lateral occipital complex of the left hemisphere adapted to the grasp-relevant dimension regardless of object size and task. Finally, the dorsal premotor cortex adapted to the grasp-relevant dimension in grasping, but not in viewing, tasks, suggesting that motor processing was complete at this stage. Taken together, our results provide a complete cortical circuit for progressive transformation of general object properties into grasp-related responses.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Mãos/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Estimulação Física , Psicofísica , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
Psychol Med ; 43(11): 2437-45, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23308393

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Criteria for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) consider impairment in instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) as exclusionary, but cross-sectional studies suggest that some high-level functional deficits are present in MCI. This longitudinal study examines informant-rated IADL in MCI, compared with cognitively normal (CN) older individuals, and explores whether functional abilities, particularly those with high cognitive demand, are predictors of MCI and dementia over a 2-year period in individuals who were CN at baseline. METHOD: A sample of 602 non-demented community dwelling individuals (375 CN and 227 with MCI) aged 70-90 years underwent baseline and 24-month assessments that included cognitive and medical assessments and an interview with a knowledgeable informant on functional abilities with the Bayer Activities of Daily Living Scale. RESULTS: Significantly more deficits in informant-reported IADL with high cognitive demand were present in MCI compared with CN individuals at baseline and 2-year follow-up. Functional ability in CN individuals at baseline, particularly in activities with high cognitive demand, predicted MCI and dementia at follow-up. Difficulties with highly cognitively demanding activities specifically predicted amnestic MCI but not non-amnestic MCI whereas those with low cognitive demand did not predict MCI or dementia. Age, depressive symptoms, cardiovascular risk factors and the sex of the informant did not contribute to the prediction. CONCLUSIONS: IADL are affected in individuals with MCI, and IADL with a high cognitive demand show impairment predating the diagnosis of MCI. Subtle cognitive impairment is therefore likely to be a major hidden burden in society.


Assuntos
Atividades Cotidianas/psicologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Demência/diagnóstico , Sintomas Prodrômicos , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Cognição , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Demência/fisiopatologia , Demência/psicologia , Depressão/psicologia , Diagnóstico Precoce , Feminino , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino
9.
Acta Psychiatr Scand ; 127(5): 394-402, 2013 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22943523

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Depression might be a risk factor for dementia. However, little is known about the prevalence of depressive symptoms in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and whether mood or motivation-related symptoms are predominant. METHOD: A total of 767 non-demented community-dwelling adults aged 70-90 years completed a comprehensive assessment, including neuropsychological testing, and a past psychiatric/medical history interview. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) and Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10). Exploratory factor analysis was performed on the GDS and K10 to derive 'mood' and 'motivation' subscales. RESULTS: A total of 290 participants were classified as having MCI and 468 as cognitively normal (CN). Participants with MCI reported more depressive symptoms, and more MCI participants met the cut-off for clinically significant symptoms, relative to CN participants. Those with amnestic MCI (aMCI), but not non-amnestic MCI, had more depressive symptoms and were more likely to meet the cut-off for clinically significant depressive symptoms, relative to CN participants. Participants with MCI reported more mood-related symptoms than CN participants, while there were no differences between groups on motivation-related symptoms. CONCLUSION: Individuals with MCI, especially aMCI, endorse more depressive symptoms when compared with cognitively intact individuals. These findings highlight the importance of assessing and treating depressive symptoms in MCI.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva/epidemiologia , Depressão/epidemiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Disfunção Cognitiva/complicações , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Estudos Transversais , Depressão/complicações , Depressão/psicologia , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , New South Wales/epidemiologia , Prevalência , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Fatores de Risco
10.
J Psychiatr Res ; 45(12): 1600-7, 2011 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21871636

RESUMO

Depressive symptoms are common in the elderly and they have been associated with cognitive and functional impairment. However, relatively less is known about the relationship of a lifetime history of depression to cognitive impairment and functional status. The aim of this cross-sectional study was to assess whether current depressive symptoms and past depression are associated with cognitive or functional impairment in a community-based sample representative of east Sydney, Australia. We also examined whether there was an interaction between current and past depression in their effects on cognitive performance. Eight hundred non-demented aged participants received a neuropsychological assessment, a past psychiatric history interview and the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale. The Bayer-Activities of Daily Living scale was completed by an informant to determine functional ability. Clinically relevant depressive symptoms were present in 6.1% of the sample and 16.6% reported a history of depression. Participants with current depression had significantly higher levels of psychological distress and anxiety, and lower life satisfaction and performed worse on memory and executive function compared to participants without current depression. After controlling for anxiety the effect on executive function was no longer significant while the effect on memory remained significant. A history of depression was associated with worse executive function, higher levels of psychological distress and anxiety, and lower life satisfaction. After controlling for psychological distress the effect of past depression on executive function was no longer significant. There were no significant interactions between current and past depression in their effects on cognitive performance. There were no differences between participants with or without current depression and with or without past depression on functional abilities. These results support the view that current and past depressive episodes are associated with poorer cognitive performance but not with functional abilities.


Assuntos
Atividades Cotidianas , Transtornos Cognitivos/complicações , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Transtorno Depressivo/complicações , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Austrália , Doenças Cardiovasculares/epidemiologia , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Estudos Transversais , Transtorno Depressivo/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Características de Residência , Estudos Retrospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Sexuais
11.
Neuroscience ; 164(3): 1284-302, 2009 Dec 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19733631

RESUMO

Coordinated eye-head gaze shifts have been evoked during electrical stimulation of the frontal cortex (supplementary eye field (SEF) and frontal eye field (FEF)) and superior colliculus (SC), but less is known about the role of lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP) in head-unrestrained gaze shifts. To explore this, two monkeys (M1 and M2) were implanted with recording chambers and 3-D eye+ head search coils. Tungsten electrodes delivered trains of electrical pulses (usually 200 ms duration) to and around area LIP during head-unrestrained gaze fixations. A current of 200 muA consistently evoked small, short-latency contralateral gaze shifts from 152 sites in M1 and 243 sites in M2 (Constantin et al., 2007). Gaze kinematics were independent of stimulus amplitude and duration, except that subsequent saccades were suppressed. The average amplitude of the evoked gaze shifts was 8.46 degrees for M1 and 8.25 degrees for M2, with average head components of only 0.36 and 0.62 degrees respectively. The head's amplitude contribution to these movements was significantly smaller than in normal gaze shifts, and did not increase with behavioral adaptation. Stimulation-evoked gaze, eye and head movements qualitatively obeyed normal 3-D constraints (Donders' law and Listing's law), but with less precision. As in normal behavior, when the head was restrained LIP stimulation evoked eye-only saccades in Listing's plane, whereas when the head was not restrained, stimulation evoked saccades with position-dependent torsional components (driving the eye out of Listing's plane). In behavioral gaze-shifts, the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) then drives torsion back into Listing's plane, but in the absence of subsequent head movement the stimulation-induced torsion was "left hanging". This suggests that the position-dependent torsional saccade components are preprogrammed, and that the oculomotor system was expecting a head movement command to follow the saccade. These data show that, unlike SEF, FEF, and SC stimulation in nearly identical conditions, LIP stimulation fails to produce normally-coordinated eye-head gaze shifts.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Estimulação Elétrica , Feminino , Macaca mulatta , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Orientação/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/anatomia & histologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
12.
Exp Brain Res ; 195(3): 455-65, 2009 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19404624

RESUMO

Relatively little is known about the role of proprioception in eye-hand coordination. In a previous article (Ren et al. J Neurophysiol 96:1464-1477, 2006), we observed anisotropic (direction-dependent) saccade overshoots to hand-held targets during active, but not passive hand movements. We hypothesized that these errors arose from a limb-centered anisotropic efference copy which was transformed (uncompensated) into head coordinates for saccade control. Here, we tested this hypothesis and the role of head orientation signals in this transformation, by dissociating limb coordinates from head coordinates. Twelve human subjects made saccades to hand-held targets actively placed at eight radial locations on a frontally placed table in a dark room with four conditions: (1) right hand (body and head centered), (2) left hand (body and head centered), (3) right hand (head tilted counter-clockwise), (4) right hand (head tilted clockwise). In condition 1, we observed the same anisotropic pattern of overshooting errors-approximately along the axis of the forearm-that we reported previously. Overall, these amplitude errors were much smaller for the left hand. However, the anisotropic pattern was observed for both hands, but reversed symmetrically between the right versus left hand. Head tilt did not cause any systematic errors in saccade direction. Moreover, during head tilt, the anisotropic amplitude errors-while showing some distortions-did not rotate with the head. These findings suggest that transformations of somatosensory information into oculomotor coordinates account for head orientation, and that the anisotropic amplitude errors in hand-guided saccades arise in limb coordinates.


Assuntos
Mãos , Destreza Motora , Propriocepção , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Cabeça , Humanos , Masculino , Postura , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
13.
J Neurophysiol ; 100(4): 2005-14, 2008 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18684904

RESUMO

Posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has been implicated in the integration of visual and proprioceptive information for the planning of action. We previously reported that single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over dorsal-lateral PPC perturbs the early stages of spatial processing for memory-guided reaching. However, our data did not distinguish whether TMS disrupted the reach goal or the internal estimate of initial hand position needed to calculate the reach vector. To test between these hypotheses, we investigated reaching in six healthy humans during left and right parietal TMS while varying visual feedback of the movement. We reasoned that if TMS were disrupting the internal representation of hand position, visual feedback from the hand might still recalibrate this signal. We tested four viewing conditions: 1) final vision of hand position; 2) full vision of hand position; 3) initial and final vision of hand position; and 4) middle and final vision of hand position. During the final vision condition, left parietal stimulation significantly increased endpoint variability, whereas right parietal stimulation produced a significant leftward shift in both visual fields. However, these errors significantly decreased with visual feedback of the hand during both planning and control stages of the reach movement. These new findings demonstrate that 1) visual feedback of hand position during the planning and early execution of the reach can recalibrate the perturbed signal and, importantly, and 2) TMS over dorsal-lateral PPC does not disrupt the internal representation of the visual goal, but rather the reach vector, or more likely the sense of initial hand position that is used to calculate this vector.


Assuntos
Mãos/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto , Braço/inervação , Braço/fisiologia , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Dominância Ocular/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Mãos/inervação , Humanos , Campos Visuais/fisiologia
14.
J Neurophysiol ; 98(2): 696-709, 2007 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17553952

RESUMO

Previous studies suggest that stimulation of lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP) evokes saccadic eye movements toward eye- or head-fixed goals, whereas most single-unit studies suggest that LIP uses an eye-fixed frame with eye-position modulations. The goal of our study was to determine the reference frame for gaze shifts evoked during LIP stimulation in head-unrestrained monkeys. Two macaques (M1 and M2) were implanted with recording chambers over the right intraparietal sulcus and with search coils for recording three-dimensional eye and head movements. The LIP region was microstimulated using pulse trains of 300 Hz, 100-150 microA, and 200 ms. Eighty-five putative LIP sites in M1 and 194 putative sites in M2 were used in our quantitative analysis throughout this study. Average amplitude of the stimulation-evoked gaze shifts was 8.67 degrees for M1 and 7.97 degrees for M2 with very small head movements. When these gaze-shift trajectories were rotated into three coordinate frames (eye, head, and body), gaze endpoint distribution for all sites was most convergent to a common point when plotted in eye coordinates. Across all sites, the eye-centered model provided a significantly better fit compared with the head, body, or fixed-vector models (where the latter model signifies no modulation of the gaze trajectory as a function of initial gaze position). Moreover, the probability of evoking a gaze shift from any one particular position was modulated by the current gaze direction (independent of saccade direction). These results provide causal evidence that the motor commands from LIP encode gaze command in eye-fixed coordinates but are also subtly modulated by initial gaze position.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Feminino , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Imageamento Tridimensional/métodos , Macaca mulatta , Probabilidade , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 182(2): 189-98, 2007 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17551720

RESUMO

We previously showed that saccades tend to overshoot briefly flashed targets that were manually displaced in the dark (Ren et al. 2006). However it was not clear if the overshoot originated from a sensory error in measuring hand displacement or from a premotor error in saccade programming, because gaze and hand position started at the same central position. Here, we tested between these hypotheses by dissociating the initial eye and hand position. Five hand/target positions (center, far, near, right, left) on a frontally-placed horizontal surface were used in four paradigms: Center or Peripheral Eye-hand Association (CA or PA, both gaze and right hand started from the center or a same peripheral location) and Hand or Eye Dissociation (HD or ED, hand or gaze started from one of three non-target peripheral locations). Subjects never received any visual feedback about the final target location and the subjects' hand displacement. In the CA paradigm, subjects showed the same overshoot that we showed previously. However, changing both initial eye and hand positions relative to the final target (PA) affected the pattern, significantly altering the directions of overshoots. Changing only the initial position of hand (HD) did not have this effect, whereas changing only initial eye position (ED) had the same effect as the PA condition (CA approximately HD, PA approximately ED). Furthermore, multiple regression analysis showed that the direction of the ideal saccade contributed significantly to the endpoint direction error, not the direction of the hand path. These results suggest that these errors do not primarily arise from misestimates of the hand trajectory, but rather from a process of comparing the initial eye position and the limb proprioceptive signal during saccade programming.


Assuntos
Mãos , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 96(6): 3016-27, 2006 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17005619

RESUMO

Dorsal posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has been implicated through single-unit recordings, neuroimaging data, and studies of brain-damaged humans in the spatial guidance of reaching and pointing movements. The present study examines the causal effect of single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the left and right dorsal posterior parietal cortex during a memory-guided "reach-to-touch" movement task in six human subjects. Stimulation of the left parietal hemisphere significantly increased endpoint variability, independent of visual field, with no horizontal bias. In contrast, right parietal stimulation did not increase variability, but instead produced a significantly systematic leftward directional shift in pointing (contralateral to stimulation site) in both visual fields. Furthermore, the same lateralized pattern persisted with left-hand movement, suggesting that these aspects of parietal control of pointing movements are spatially fixed. To test whether the right parietal TMS shift occurs in visual or motor coordinates, we trained subjects to point correctly to optically reversed peripheral targets, viewed through a left-right Dove reversing prism. After prism adaptation, the horizontal pointing direction for a given visual target reversed, but the direction of shift during right parietal TMS did not reverse. Taken together, these data suggest that induction of a focal current reveals a hemispheric asymmetry in the early stages of the putative spatial processing in PPC. These results also suggest that a brief TMS pulse modifies the output of the right PPC in motor coordinates downstream from the adapted visuomotor reversal, rather than modifying the upstream visual coordinates of the memory representation.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Adulto , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia
17.
J Neurophysiol ; 96(3): 1464-77, 2006 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16707717

RESUMO

The saccade generator updates memorized target representations for saccades during eye and head movements. Here, we tested if proprioceptive feedback from the arm can also update handheld object locations for saccades, and what intrinsic coordinate system(s) is used in this transformation. We measured radial saccades beginning from a central light-emitting diode to 16 target locations arranged peripherally in eight directions and two eccentricities on a horizontal plane in front of subjects. Target locations were either indicated 1) by a visual flash, 2) by the subject actively moving the handheld central target to a peripheral location, 3) by the experimenter passively moving the subject's hand, or 4) through a combination of the above proprioceptive and visual stimuli. Saccade direction was relatively accurate, but subjects showed task-dependent systematic overshoots and variable errors in radial amplitude. Visually guided saccades showed the smallest overshoot, followed by saccades guided by both vision and proprioception, whereas proprioceptively guided saccades showed the largest overshoot. In most tasks, the overall distribution of saccade endpoints was shifted and expanded in a gaze- or head-centered cardinal coordinate system. However, the active proprioception task produced a tilted pattern of errors, apparently weighted toward a limb-centered coordinate system. This suggests the saccade generator receives an efference copy of the arm movement command but fails to compensate for the arm's inertia-related directional anisotropy. Thus the saccade system is able to transform hand-centered somatosensory signals into oculomotor coordinates and combine somatosensory signals with visual inputs, but it seems to have a poorly calibrated internal model of limb properties.


Assuntos
Memória/fisiologia , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Movimentos da Cabeça , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção Visual
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 169(4): 532-48, 2006 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16374631

RESUMO

Transsaccadic integration (TSI) refers to the perceptual integration of visual information collected across separate gaze fixations. Current theories of TSI disagree on whether it relies solely on visual algorithms or also uses extra-retinal signals. We designed a task in which subjects had to rely on internal oculomotor signals to synthesize remembered stimulus features presented within separate fixations. Using a mouse-controlled pointer, subjects estimated the intersection point of two successively presented bars, in the dark, under two conditions: Saccade task (bars viewed in separate fixations) and Fixation task (bars viewed in one fixation). Small, but systematic biases were observed in both intersection tasks, including position-dependent vertical undershoots and order-dependent horizontal biases. However, the magnitude of these errors was statistically indistinguishable in the Saccade and Fixation tasks. Moreover, part of the errors in the Saccade task were dependent on saccade metrics, showing that egocentric oculomotor signals were used to fuse remembered location and orientation features across saccades. We hypothesize that these extra-retinal signals are normally used to reduce the computational load of calculating visual correspondence between fixations. We further hypothesize that TSI may be implemented within dynamically updated recurrent feedback loops that interconnect a common eye-centered map in occipital cortex with both the "dorsal" and "ventral" streams of visual analysis.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Análise de Componente Principal
19.
Nat Neurosci ; 8(4): 418-20, 2005 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15768034

RESUMO

Optic ataxia is a disorder associated with posterior parietal lobe lesions, in which visually guided reaching errors typically occur for peripheral targets. It has been assumed that these errors are related to a faulty sensorimotor transformation of inputs from the 'ataxic visual field'. However, we show here that the errors observed in the contralesional field in optic ataxia depend on a dynamic gaze-centered internal representation of reach space.


Assuntos
Ataxia/fisiopatologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroculografia/métodos , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Orientação , Lobo Parietal/lesões , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo , Campos Visuais/fisiologia
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 92(1): 10-9, 2004 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15212434

RESUMO

Eye-hand coordination is complex because it involves the visual guidance of both the eyes and hands, while simultaneously using eye movements to optimize vision. Since only hand motion directly affects the external world, eye movements are the slave in this system. This eye-hand visuomotor system incorporates closed-loop visual feedback but here we focus on early feedforward mechanisms that allow primates to make spatially accurate reaches. First, we consider how the parietal cortex might store and update gaze-centered representations of reach targets during a sequence of gaze shifts and fixations. Recent evidence suggests that such representations might be compared with hand position signals within this early gaze-centered frame. However, the resulting motor error commands cannot be treated independently of their frame of origin or the frame of their destined motor command. Behavioral experiments show that the brain deals with the nonlinear aspects of such reference frame transformations, and incorporates internal models of the complex linkage geometry of the eye-head-shoulder system. These transformations are modeled as a series of vector displacement commands, rotated by eye and head orientation, and implemented between parietal and frontal cortex through efficient parallel neuronal architectures. Finally, we consider how this reach system might interact with the visually guided grasp system through both parallel and coordinated neural algorithms.


Assuntos
Movimento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...